Girish Mahajan (Editor)

Shabo language

Updated on
Edit
Like
Comment
Share on FacebookTweet on TwitterShare on LinkedInShare on Reddit
Pronunciation
  
tʃʼabu, ʃʼabu

Region
  
western SNNPR

Native speakers
  
400 (2000)

Native to
  
Ethiopia

Ethnicity
  
600 Shabo (2000)

Language family
  
likely isolate; possibly relatable to Komuz

Shabo (or preferably Chabu; also called Mikeyir) is an endangered language and likely language isolate spoken by about 400 former hunter-gatherers in southwestern Ethiopia, in the westernmost part of the Southern Nations, Nationalities, and People's Region. They live in three places in the Keficho Shekicho Zone: Anderaccha, Gecha, and Kaabo. As they shift from hunting and gathering to more settled agriculture and to working as laborers, many of its speakers are shifting to other neighboring languages, in particular Majang language and Shekkacho (Mocha); its vocabulary is heavily influenced by loanwords from both these languages, particularly Majangir, as well as Amharic. Its classification is uncertain, though it appears to be a Nilo-Saharan language (Anbessa & Unseth 1989, Fleming 1991, Blench 2010). It was first reported to be a separate language by Lionel Bender in 1977, based on data gathered by missionary Harvey Hoekstra. A grammar was published in 2015 (Kibebe 2015).

Contents

Classification

Once the many loanwords from its immediate neighbors, Majang and Shakicho, are removed, the wordlists collected show a significant number of Koman words side by side with a larger number of words with no obvious external relationships. The tentative grammar so far collected offers few obviously convincing external similarities. On this basis, Fleming (1991) has classified Shabo as Nilo-Saharan and, within Nilo-Saharan, as nearest to Koman. Anbessa & Unseth consider it Nilo-Saharan, but present little by way of argument for their position, and no detail on its position within the family. Schnoebelen (2009) in his phylogenetic analysis says that Shabo is best treated as an isolate, but does not exclude the possibility of contradicting evidence gained from applying the comparative method (which still needs to be done); Kibebe (2015) evaluates Schnoebelen as the most rigorous comparison to date. Blench (2010) maintains that Shabo does pattern with the Nilo-Saharan family, and that recent data on Gumuz helped tie the languages together.

Phonology

The consonants are:

Consonants in parentheses are not entirely phonemic according to Teferra (1995):

  • [p] and [f] are in free variation
  • [s] and [ʃ], and sometimes also [c], [ɟ], and [ʒ], are in free variation, as in Majang; Teferra speculatively links this to the traditional practice of removing the lower incisors of men.
  • [h] and [k] occasionally alternate.
  • Implosive consonants are common in languages of the area, but ejective consonants are not found in Majang.

    Consonant length is found in several words, such as walla "goat", kutti "knee"; however, it is often unstable.

    Teferra tentatively postulates 9 vowels: /i/ /ɨ/ /u/ /e/ /ə/ /o/ /ɛ/ /a/ /ɔ/, possibly with further distinctions based on advanced tongue root. Five of these, /a/ /e/ /i/ /o/ /u/, have long counterparts. Occasionally final vowels are deleted, shortening medial vowels: e.g. deego or deg "crocodile".

    The syllable structure is (C)V(C); all consonants except /pʼ/ and /tʼ/ can occur syllable-finally.

    The language is tonal, but its tonology is unclear. Two minimal pairs are cited by Teferra 1995, including "kill" versus "meat".

    Syntax

    Basic word order is subject–object–verb; there are postpositions rather than prepositions.

    Pronouns

    Shabo has an unusually complex pronoun system for Africa:

    The pronouns "I" and "he" have been compared to Surmic languages; however, there are also resemblances in the pronouns with the Gunza language (Bender 1983). The gender distinctions made are unusual for Africa.

    Verbs

    Negation is by adding the particle be after the verb or noun negated: gumu be "(it is) not (a) stick", ʔam be-gea "he will not come" ("come not-?"). Negative forms in b are widespread in Nilo-Saharan and Afro-Asiatic languages.

    There appears to be a causative suffix -ka: mawo hoop "water boiled" → upa mawo hoop-ka "(a) man boiled water".

    A particle git (infinitive? subjunctive?) marks the verb in constructions with "want": moopa git inɗeet ("sit git want") "I want to sit".

    Much of the verbal morphology is uncertain; there appears to be a 3rd person singular future suffix -g- (e.g. inɗage t'a-g "he will eat") and a 2nd person plural suffix -ɗe (e.g. subuk maakɛle kak t'a-ɗe "You (pl.) ate corn", "you-pl. corn past? eat-2nd-pl.")

    Nouns

    Plurals are optional; when used, they are formed with a word yɛɛro afterwards.

    There is a suffix -ka which sometimes mark the direct object, e.g. upa kaan-ik ye "a man saw a dog" ("man dog saw"), but also has many other uses. A similar suffix is found in many Eastern Sudanic languages, but there is it specifically accusative.

    Postpositions

    Shabo uses postpositions after nouns, e.g.: upa mana pond ɗɛpik moi "a man sat on a rock" (lit. "man rock on ? sat").

    Numbers

    The number system, as given by Tefera and Unseth, is as follows, with Majang equivalents to show how much is borrowed:

    1. iŋki (Majang om-oŋ)
    2. bap (pɛɛy)
    3. jiita (jiit)
    4. aŋan (aŋan)
    5. tuul (tuul)
    6. tulu(ŋ/m) (tuul a om)
    7. tulikakiŋki (possibly error for 6?) (tuul a pɛɛy)
    8. tunajiita (tuul a jiit)
    9. tulaaŋan (tuul a aŋan)
    10. bapif (bap if = "two hands") (aarn = 'two hands')
    11. mabafifiŋki (aarn a om)

    and 20 is iŋk upa kor ("one person complete") cf. Majang rumer iɗit 'one person complete'.

    Sample sentences

    mawo hoop: water boiled upa mawo hoop-ka: A man boiled water (lit. "man water boiled-caus.") gumu be: it is not a stick (lit. "stick not".) ma gumu: it is a stick (lit. "stick ?") dɛrbakan kaal nu ɗe-be: Derbakan does not have a dog (lit. "Derbakan dog poss.? ?:not") dɛrbakan kaal nu yaaŋk: Derbakan has a dog (lit. "Derbakan dog poss.? positive?") ʔam be-gea: he will not come (lit. "come not-?") inɗigi am-k: he will come (lit. "? come ?") tin-ta be-ge: he will not eat (lit. "? eat not ?") inɗage t'a-g: he will eat (lit. "? eat ?") paar bap: two snakes (lit. "snake two") upa kaan-ik ye: a man saw a dog (lit. "man dog-obj. saw") kaan upa-k ye: a dog saw a man (lit. "dog man-obj. saw") koto upa dɛpik ye: a woman saw a man (lit. "woman man tense? saw") gom c'uwa t'a: fire burned wood (lit. "fire wood ate") cu ɗɛpik ibalabiyan-an ɗe (word divisions uncertain): you (pl.) came (lit. "you(pl.) ?:? come-2pl.") subuk maakɛle kak t'a-ɗe: you (pl.) ate corn (lit. "you(pl.) corn aux? eat-2pl.") wo ka git inɗeet: I want to drink (lit. "drink ? infin.? want") moopa git inɗeet: I want to sit (lit. "sit ? infin.? want") abiyaŋge: they came upa kakaak jaal kaki ye ʔam: I saw the man who came yesterday (lit. "man came yesterday ? saw ?") upa mana pond ɗɛpik moi: a man sat on a rock (lit. "man rock on aux.? sat")

    References

    Shabo language Wikipedia