Puneet Varma (Editor)

Debuccalization

Updated on
Edit
Like
Comment
Share on FacebookTweet on TwitterShare on LinkedInShare on Reddit

Debuccalization is a sound change in which an oral consonant loses its original place of articulation and moves it to the glottis (usually [h], [ɦ], or [ʔ]). The pronunciation of a consonant as [h] is sometimes called aspiration but in phonetics, aspiration is the burst of air accompanying a stop. The word comes from Latin bucca, meaning "cheek".

Contents

Debuccalization is usually seen as a sub-type of lenition, often defined as a consonant mutation involving the weakening of a consonant by progressive shifts in pronunciation.

Debuccalization processes occur in many different types of environments such as the following:

  • word-initially, as in Kannada
  • word-finally, as in Burmese
  • intervocalically, as in a number of English varieties (e.g. litter [ˈlɪʔə])
  • Arabic

    /q/ is debuccalized to /ʔ/ in several Arabic varieties, such as Egyptian Arabic.

    British and American English

    Most English-speakers in England and many speakers of American English debuccalize /t/ to a glottal stop [ʔ] in two environments: in word-final position before another consonant

  • get ready [ˈɡɛʔˈɹɛɾi]
  • not much [ˈnɑʔˈmʌtʃ]
  • not good [ˈnɑʔˈɡʊd]
  • it says [ɪʔˈsɛz]
  • and before a syllabic [n̩] following /l/, /r/, /n/, or a vowel. Here the /t/ may also be nasally released.

  • Milton [ˈmɪlʔn̩]
  • Martin [ˈmɑɹʔn̩]
  • mountain [ˈmaʊnʔn̩]
  • cotton [ˈkɑʔn̩]
  • Cockney English

    In Cockney English, /t/ is often realized as a glottal stop [ʔ] between vowels, liquids, and nasals (notably in the word bottle), a process called t-glottalization.

    Scottish English

    Th-debuccalization occurs in some varieties of Scottish English, particularly on the West coast e.g. /θɪn/ is realised as [hɪn] in said varieties.

    German

    The Bavarian dialect debuccalizes /p, t, k, b, d, ɡ/ that occur between two consonants (a situation often produced by vowel elision in the same dialect) and realizes them as [ʔ]. Thus Antn (ducks) and Andn (Andes) are both pronounced [ˈɑnʔn̩], but speakers believe them to be [ˈɑntn̩] and [ˈɑndn̩], respectively. Frequency depending on the location; hàn(d) ("are") occurs instead of the other (and more common) Bavarian form sàn(d) (from the German seind, in contemporary German: sind).

    Slavey

    All coda consonants in Slavey must be glottal. When a non-glottal consonant would otherwise be positioned in a syllable coda, it debuccalizes to [h]:

  • /ts’ad/[ts’ah] ('hat')
  • /xaz/[xah] ('scar')
  • /tl’ulʒ/[tl’uh] ('rope')
  • Scottish English

    In some varieties of Scottish English, a non word-final /θ/ th shifted to [h], a process called th-debuccalization.

    Proto-Greek

    In Proto-Greek, /s/ shifted to [h] initially and between sonorants (vowels, liquids, and nasals).

  • Proto-Indo-European *septḿ̥ → Ancient Greek heptá "seven" (vs. Latin septem)
  • Intervocalic /h/ was lost by the time of Ancient Greek, and vowels in hiatus were contracted in the Attic dialect.

  • post-PIE *ǵénesos → Proto-Greek *génehos → Ionic géneos : Attic génous "of a race"
  • Before a liquid or nasal, an /h/ was assimilated to the preceding vowel in Attic-Ionic and Doric and to the following nasal in Aeolic. The process is also described as loss of /h/ and subsequent lengthening of a vowel or consonant to keep the syllable the same length (compensatory lengthening).

  • PIE *h₁ésmi → Proto-Greek *ehmi → Attic-Ionic ēmí : Aeolic émmi "I am"
  • Sanskrit

    In Sanskrit, /s/ becomes [h] (written in transliteration) when utterance-final, e.g., kā́mas "erotic love" becomes kā́maḥ.

    Spanish

    A number of Spanish dialects debuccalize /s/ at the end of a syllable to [h] or [ɦ].

    Galician

    In many varieties of Galician as well as in Galician-influenced Spanish, the phoneme /g/ may debuccalize (gheada) to [h] in most or all instances, though [x] and [ħ] are also possible realizations. There is also an inverse hypercorrection process of older or less educated Galician speakers replacing the phoneme /x/ of the Spanish language by [g], what is called gueada.

    Portuguese

    Portuguese is affected to a much lesser degree by debuccalization, but it occurs, being specially notorious in its Brazilian variety.

    All over Brazil, the phoneme /ʁ/ (historically an alveolar trill /r/ that moved to an uvular position by both French and Arab-Berber influence) has a rather long inventory of allophones, or up to [r ɻ̝̊ ç x ɣ χ ʁ ʀ ħ h ɦ], with all but [ɣ] being common. Few dialects, such as sulista and fluminense, give preference to voiced allophones; elsewhere, they are common only as coda, before voiced consonants.

    In such dialects, especially among people speaking an educated variety of Portuguese, it is usual for the rhotic coda in the syllable rhyme to be an alveolar tap, as in European Portuguese and many registers of Spanish, or to be realized as [χ] or [x]. In the rest of the country, it is generally realized as [h], even among speakers that do not have this allophone as the dominant, or deleted entirely, as commonplace in the Vernacular.

    But in some mineiro and mineiro-influenced fluminense rural registers, what changes is that [h] is used in the rhyme, but as an allophone of /l/ (while rhotic consonants are most often deleted in the rhyme), a mar-mal merger, instead of the much more common and less stigmatized mau-mal merger characteristic of all Brazilian urban centers with the exception of those bordering Mercosur countries, where coda [ɫ] was preserved, and the entire North and Northeast regions. It comes from the process of replacing Amerindian languages and línguas gerais by Portuguese, what created [ɹ], [ɻ] and r-colored vowel as allophones of both /ɾ/ (now mostly /ʁ/) and /l/ (now mostly [ ~ ʊ̯]) phonemes in the coda since their native speakers had difficulty with reproducing them (caipira dialect). Later Portuguese influence from other regions made those allophones get rarer in some areas, but the mar-mal merger did not disappear in such few isolated villages and towns.

    Finally, many fluminense registers, specially those of the poor and of the youth, most northern and northeastern dialects, and to a much minor degree all other Brazilian dialects, debuccalize /s/ (that is, [ɕ ~ ʑ]), but not in the strength of what is done with Spanish. Still, there is a mar-mas merger or even a mar-mais merger: mas mesmo assim "but even so" or mas mesma, sim "though, right, the same (f) one" [mɐɦ ˈmeɦmə ˈsĩ]; mais light "lighter, more slim", or also "less caloric/fatty" [ˈmaɦ ˈlajtɕ]; mas de mim, não "but from me, no" or mais de mim, não "not more from me" [ˈmaɦ dʑi ˈmĩ ˈnɜ̃w]. A coda rhotic in the Brazilian dialects spoken in the Centro-Sul area is generally not glottal, with few exceptions, and a debuccalized /s/ is rarely likely to be confused with it.

    Romanian

    In Romanian, Moldavian dialect, /f/ becomes [h]; să fie becomes să hie etc. as happened in Old Spanish, Old Gascon and Old Japanese.

    Gaelic

    In Scottish and Irish Gaelic, s and t changed by lenition to [h], spelled sh and th.

    Loanwords

    Debuccalization can be a feature of loanword phonology. For example, while Korean allows certain coda obstruents, Japanese does not. Those consonants realized in Korean as unreleased voiceless stops ([p˺ t˺ k˺]) may be realized in Japanese as glottal stops:

  • [tɕuk˺][tɕuʔ] ('porridge')
  • [mok˺][moʔ] ('neck')
  • [mat˺][maʔ] ('flavor')
  • [tap˺][taʔ] ('tower')
  • Similarly, debuccalization can be seen in Bahasa Indonesia loans into Selayarese.

    References

    Debuccalization Wikipedia