Trisha Shetty (Editor)

Three degrees of influence

Updated on
Edit
Like
Comment
Share on FacebookTweet on TwitterShare on LinkedInShare on Reddit

Three Degrees of Influence is a theory in the realm of social networks, proposed by Nicholas A. Christakis and James H. Fowler in 2007. Christakis and Fowler found that social networks have great influence on individuals' behavior. But social influence does not end with the people to whom a person is directly tied. We influence our friends, who in their turn influence their friends, and so our actions can influence people we have never met, to whom we are only indirectly tied. They posit that diverse phenomena "ripple through our network, having an impact on our friends (one degree), our friends’ friends (two degrees), and even our friends’ friends’ friends (three degrees). Our influence gradually dissipates and ceases to have a noticeable effect on people beyond the social frontier that lies at three degrees of separation".

Contents

This argument is basically that peer effects need not stop at one degree, and that, if we can affect our friends, then we can (in many cases) affect our friends' friends, and so on. However, across a broad set of empirical settings, using both observational and experimental methods, they observed that the effect seems to no longer be meaningful at a social horizon of three degrees.

Christakis and Fowler examined phenomena from various domains, such as obesity, happiness, cooperation, and politics; investigations by other groups have subsequently explored many other phenomena in this way (including crime, social learning, etc.).

Rationale

Influence dissipates after three degrees (to and from friends’ friends’ friends) for three reasons, Christakis and Fowler propose:

  1. Intrinsic decay - corruption of information (like the game telephone).
  2. Network instability - social ties become unstable (or are not constant across time) at more than three degrees of separation.
  3. Evolutionary purpose - we evolved in small groups where everyone was connected by three degrees or fewer (an idea receiving subsequent support ).

Scientific literature

Initial studies using observational data by Christakis and Fowler suggested that a variety of attributes—like obesity, smoking, and happiness—rather than being individualistic are casually correlated by contagion mechanisms that transmit such phenomena over long distances within social networks. Certain subsequent analyses explored limitations to these analyses (subject to different statistical assumptions); or expressed concern that the statistical methods employed in these analyses could not fully control for other environmental factors; or resulted in statistical estimates without straightforward interpretations; or did not fully account for homophily processes in the creation and retention of relationships over time;.

But other scholarship using sensitivity analysis has found that the basic estimates regarding the transmissibility of obesity and smoking cessation, for example, are robust, or has otherwise replicated or supported the findings. Additional detailed modeling work published in 2016 showed that the GEE modeling approach used by Christakis and Fowler, and others, was quite effective at estimating social contagion effects, and in distinguishing them from homophily. This paper concluded, "For network influence, we find that the approach appears to have excellent sensitivity, and quite good specificity with regard to distinguishing the presence or absence of such a “network effect”, regardless of whether or not homophily is present in network formation. This was true for small cohorts (n = 30) and larger cohorts (n = 1000), and for cohorts that displayed lesser and greater realism in their distribution of friendships." Another methodological paper, by physicists ver Steeg and Galstyan, suggests it is indeed possible to bound estimates of peer effects even given the modeling constraints faced by Christakis and Fowler and even if parametric assumptions are otherwise required to identify such effects using observational data (if substantial unobserved homophily is thought to be present).

Christakis and Fowler reviewed critical and supportive findings regarding the three degrees of influence phenomenon and the analytic approaches used to discern it in 2013. Additional support for the modeling approach used by Christakis and Fowler provided by other authors has continued to appear, including of the three-degrees-of-influence property. And additional analytic approaches to observational data have also been supportive, including matched sample estimation, and reshuffling techniques (validating the edge directionality test proposed by Christakis and Fowler in their 2007 paper as an identification strategy). The three degrees of influence property has also been noted, by other groups, using observational data regarding criminal networks.

In addition, subsequent studies (by many research groups, including Christakis and Fowler) have found strong causal evidence of behavioral contagion processes (including those that spread beyond dyads, out to two, three, or four degrees) using randomized controlled experiments. including one experiment involving 61,000,000 people that showed spread of voting behavior out to two degrees of separation. A 2014 paper also confirmed the spread of emotions beyond dyads, as proposed in 2008 by Christakis and Fowler, online, using another massive experiment. Another experimental study evaluated the spread of risk perception, and documented inflection at approximately three degrees.

Moral implications

The idea of network influence raises the question of free will, because it suggests that we are influenced by factors which we cannot control and which we are not aware of. Christakis and Fowler claim (in their book, Connected) that policy makers should use the knowledge about social networks in order to create a better society with a more efficient public policy. This applies to many aspects of life, from public health to economics. For instance, they note that it might be preferable to immunize individuals located in the center of a network in preference to structurally peripheral individuals. Or, it might be much more effective to motivate clusters or people to avoid criminal behavior than to act upon individuals or than to punish each criminal separately.

If people are connected to everyone by six degrees of separation (according to the social psychologist Stanley Milgram) and influence those up to three degrees (Christakis and Fowler), then people can reach "halfway" to anyone in the world.

References

Three degrees of influence Wikipedia